Original scientific article
Does colour matter? Preference of mice for different colours of the house mouse igloo: an observational study
by Karen Gjendala, Jan Lund Ottesenb, Dorte Bratbo Sørensena
aSection of Experimental Animal Models, Faculty of Health and Medical Sciences, University of Copenhagen, Groennegaardsvej 15, DK-1870 Frederiksberg C, Denmark
bNovo Nordisk A/S, Novo Nordisk Park, DK-2760 Måløv, Denmark
Correspondence: Karen Gjendal, Section of
Experimental Animal Models,
Faculty of Health and Medical Sciences, University of Copenhagen,
Groennegaardsvej 15, DK-1870 Frederiksberg C, Denmark
E-mail address: kgjendal@sund.ku.dk
Tel: +45 35336940, Denmark
Summary
It is argued that mice have poor colour vision and are insensitive to
the colour red, which they in theory perceive as dark. Therefore, the
red tinted Bio-Serv Mouse Igloo® is used to provide mice with a dark
shelter while enabling humans to monitor the animals through the
shelter without disturbing them. However, we do not know if mice like
the colour red. Therefore, this study investigated whether mice prefer
an amber or blue igloo over a red igloo, still making it possible for
humans to see through the igloos. A preference test consisting of 3
cages each containing a different coloured igloo, connected to a
barren central cage, was conducted for 11 h 20 min in 32 female B6
mice. The results showed that some female B6 mice did not like to use
the Bio-Serv Mouse Igloo® as a shelter, regardless of colour. Almost
half of the mice chose to stay outside the igloos, but when choosing
an igloo the mice preferred the blue or amber igloo over the red
igloo.
Introduction
As a nocturnal, burrowing species, mice have a different visual capacity to humans. Unlike humans, mice have two and not three types of cone photopigment with maximal sensitivities around 359 nm (UV cone) and 511 nm (M cone), respectively (Huberman and Niell 2011; Jacobs et al. 1991; Yokoyama et al. 1998). Also, mice are regarded as being insensitive to the colour red, which has wavelengths ranging from 622 to 780 nm (Huberman and Niell 2011; Key 2004; NASA 2017). In other words, it is believed that mice will perceive red as being dark.
The Bio-Serv Mouse Igloo™ can be used as a shelter, a running
wheel-igloo and in the scoring of nesting behaviour (Howerton et al.
2008; Key 2004; Pedersen et al. 2014; Robertson and Rowland 2005;
Sager et al. 2010). The igloo is transparent and is available in red,
amber and blue. These igloos provide shelter for the animals and easy
visibility for animal caretakers, and the red igloo is believed to be
perceived as dark by mice. Therefore, a transparent red-coloured
shelter seems ideal for laboratory mice. But do the mice like these
types of shelter? Marques and Olsson (2007) found that mice stopped
using a red coloured Tecniplast Mouse House® shelter for sleeping at
the age of four to eight weeks. It is unknown whether the mice
disliked the colour or the material of the Tecniplast Mouse House.
However, in pilot studies to test the nest scoring method developed by
Pedersen et al. (2014) we found a similar dislike for the red igloo.
We assessed nests in 9 cages and noted that all the mice had built
nests using the provided Nestlet nesting material (Ancare, USA), but
in no case did the mice use the red igloo when making a nest. The
Tecniplast Mouse House® and the Mouse Igloo™ have the same red tint.
Our findings are supported by anecdotal experiences from our research
facilities indicating that mice tend to dislike and avoid using the
red mouse igloo. Furthermore, Sherwin and Glen (2003) showed that
female mice preferred white, black or green cages over red
spray-painted cages. Maybe the dark shade that mice perceive instead
of red is a shade they do not like. And maybe mice prefer an amber or
blue igloo when making a nest? It is likely that mice are capable of
seeing colours close to red. The upper limit of the murine M-cone
photopigment is close to the lower limit of the red wavelengths and
this may allow the mouse to perceive colours approaching orange.
Hence, it is possible that mice in fact do not perceive a red plastic
shelter such as the Mouse House® or the red Bio-Serv Mouse Igloo™ as
dark, as stated by e.g. Key (2004).
Therefore, in this study we investigated whether mice prefer an amber
or blue igloo over a red igloo, whilst still making it possible for
humans to see through the igloos. We hypothesized that mice prefer the
amber or blue igloo over the red igloo.
Materials and methods
The experimental procedures were approved by the Danish Animal Experiments Inspectorate under the Ministry of Environment and Food in Denmark (License number: 2016-15-0201-00871). The study was carried out in accordance with EU animal welfare requirements (Directive 2010/63/EU), and reporting of the study follows the ARRIVE guidelines for reporting animal research (Kilkenny et al. 2010). Efforts were made to minimize the number of animals used.
Animals and housing
Thirty two 6-week-old C57BL/6 female mice (Taconic, Ejby, Denmark)
were randomly allocated into 8 cages in groups of 4. The cages were of
transparent standard Makrolon type 4 (Tecniplast, Buguggiate, Italy)
(LxWxH: 540 x 320 x 180 mm) with a 70 mm raised lid. All cages
contained Aspen bedding, Enviro-Dri paper nesting material, biting
blocks (LxWxH: 50 x 10 x 10 mm) in aspen wood (all Tapvei, Harjumaa,
Estonia), two cardboard tubes (LBS serving Biotechnology, Horley,
United Kingdom) and a hemp rope (length 30cm, diameter 6mm) hanging
from the lid in the centre of the cage (Fyns Kran Udstyr, Odense,
Denmark). All cages were provided with food pellets (Altromin type
1324, Lage, Germany) and water ad libitum. The mice were maintained on
a 12:12 h dark/light cycle with lights on from 06:00 h to 18:00 h.
Room temperature was 20-220C with a relative humidity of 45-65%.
Health monitoring was performed daily and the cages were changed once
weekly.
Figure 1. Experimental setup with three cages
containing a different coloured igloo connected to a central
barren cage. Click image to enlarge |
Experimental procedure
The mice were allowed two weeks of habituation before testing. At 06.00 h two mice from the same cage were gently transported to the adjacent test room and allowed 30 min of habituation to the test room. The test room was maintained under the same conditions as the animal holding room. All testing was done during the light phase, the inactive period of mice. At 06.30 h the two mice were transferred to the experimental setup and the video cameras were turned on. All mice were tested individually to avoid the possible influence of another mouse on the igloo preference. At 17.50 h the cameras were turned off and the mice were marked with a permanent marker on the tail to make it possible to identify the remaining two mice from the same cage the following day. Each cage was tested over 2 days (4 mice in each cage, 2 mice tested per day). The experimental setup consisted of a central cage (without bedding) connected by grey plastic tubes (L 10 cm; Ø 40mm) to three preference cages (Figure 1). Each preference cage contained aspen bedding as floor substrate and ad libitum food placed in one corner (left corner closest to the connecting plastic tube). A water bottle was placed in the lid covering the central cage. The three preference cages were covered by transparent acryl covers with holes for ventilation. Each preference cage contained a different coloured Mouse Igloo (red, blue or amber) as shelter. Each day, the three coloured igloos were placed in a different preference cage (clockwise rotation) to exclude or minimize external stimuli such as light, sounds etc. affecting the shelter preference. Two sets of identical experimental setups were used, each filmed from above by a camera. The light intensity was approximately 40 lux 50 cm above the experimental setups. After each test session, the experimental setup was cleaned with 70% ethanol and the bedding was changed to remove bias caused by odours before testing the following day. Parameters assessed were: entries made into shelter, and time spent inside shelter. An entry was defined as the moment when the base of the tail was inside a shelter.
Finally, the transmission spectrum for each coloured igloo was
established using an Aligent 8453 UV-visible Spectroscopy System
(Aligent Technologies, California, USA).
Statistics
The study was performed as an observational study and no statistical analyses were applied. For each mouse, we deemed the igloo that was occupied for the longest time period as that individual’s most preferred igloo and the igloo occupied for the shortest time period as the least preferred igloo. Occupancy of the barren central cage was rare and data thereof were excluded from the analysis.
Results
One mouse escaped from the experimental setup after 2 hours and was excluded from the analysis. Also, two mice never left the first cage they entered and were therefore excluded from the analysis. Altogether, 29 mice were included in the analysis.
During the 11 h and 20 min test period, 17 mice (58.6 %) spent between
1 and 9 hours in an igloo while 12 mice (41.4 %) spent less than 1
minute or no time at all in an igloo. Of the 17 mice using an igloo, 7
preferred the blue igloo, 7 preferred the amber igloo while 3 mice
preferred the red igloo. Thus, among mice using an igloo, the majority
chose either the blue or the amber igloo over the red igloo (Figure
2). The 17 mice choosing to use an igloo spent an average of 8.03 h in
an igloo during the 11 h 20 min test time. This means that these mice
spent 71 % of the test time inside an igloo; 3.29 h were spent in the
blue igloo, 3.32 h in the amber igloo and 1.42 h in the red igloo
(Figure 3). Hence 133 % more time was spent in the blue or amber igloo
compared with the red igloo. Also, among the 12 mice not using an
igloo at all, 5 mice chose to be in the cage containing the blue igloo
and thereby in close proximity to the blue igloo, 5 mice chose the
cage with the amber igloo while only 2 mice chose the cage containing
the red igloo. The 17 mice using an igloo entered on average the blue
igloo 60 times, the red igloo 57 times and the amber igloo 49 times.
The transmission spectra for the blue, red and amber igloos are
depicted in Figure 4 together with the relative sensitivity of the two
known murine photopigments. The transmission of the blue igloo peaks
at 495 nm while the transmission of the red and amber igloos peaks at
657 nm and 604 nm, respectively (Figure 4). From Figure 4 it is clear
that the red igloo is a visible object for mice, but the red igloo
only reflects a small amount of light detectable by the murine
photoreceptors.
Figure 4. Preference of female B6 mice for
igloos of different colours. 29 female B6 mice were included. 17
mice spent between 1 and 9 hours in an igloo while 12 mice used
an igloo less than a minute or not at all. Click image to enlarge |
Discussion
In this study investigating the preference of female B6 mice for
different coloured igloos, almost half of the mice did not use an
igloo as a shelter. Among mice choosing to stay in an igloo, the
majority preferred the blue or amber igloo over the red igloo. Also,
among mice choosing not to stay in an igloo, the majority preferred to
stay in cages containing a blue or an amber igloo over the cage
containing a red igloo.
The results from our study indicate that female B6 mice either choose
to use the igloos for shelter for an extended time period or not use
them at all. Almost half of the mice included in the analysis chose to
stay outside an igloo even though they did not have any alternative
shelter or nesting material. The other half stayed inside an igloo for
an average of 8.03 h, and the majority of these mice preferred to stay
inside the blue or amber coloured igloo. This indicates that mice
using an igloo for shelter rarely chose the red igloo. Even though the
mice preferred the blue or amber igloo over the red igloo, they still
entered all igloos approximately the same number of times. Mice have
an innate preference for dark enclosed spaces. In theory, the red
igloo represents one as mice are regarded to be insensitive to red
colours (Key 2004; Latham and Mason 2004). Therefore, mice are
expected to like and prefer the red coloured igloo. Also, the igloo
itself represents an enclosed area and this gives the mice an
opportunity to escape the high intensity light and avoid damage to
their retinas. However, our results show that the red igloo used in
this study is in fact a visible object for the murine eye. Although,
the red igloo reflects only a minor portion of light detectable by the
murine photoreceptors. As mentioned, almost half of the mice in this
study did not like the igloos, but when they chose an igloo, they
chose the blue or the amber over the red igloo despite the common
belief that mice like red shelters. A reason for this could be the
small amount of light that mice detect through the red igloo. Maybe
mice do not like the shades of red they are able to see. Also, it
cannot be ruled out that the 30 min habituation period to the test
room was too short and that the outcome therefore was affected by this
even though we transported the mice gently and only to the adjacent
room. It is a common statement that even short periods of
transportation affect mice and that 24 h are needed for
acclimatization to a novel environment. However, data supporting this
are very limited. Also, reproducing the results of this study has not
been attempted and it is therefore not possible to know whether the
results of this study are completely reliable. This could be a goal
for future studies.
Other studies have shown that mice prefer other types of shelters over
a red plastic shelter and that mice prefer to be in a black or green
cage rather than a red cage (Marques and Olsson 2007; Sherwin and Glen
2003; Van Loo et al. 2005). Moreover, a recent study by Wren-Dali et
al. (2016) demonstrated that coloured tunnels serving as enrichment
altered the melatonin levels in rats. Additionally, several other
studies have shown that different coloured cages, and even a dim red
light in the ceiling, affect hormone levels in rats (Dauchy et al.
2013a, 2013b, 2015; Wren et al. 2014). It is difficult to know how
mice perceive the colours at the upper limit of the murine M-opsin.
However, according to previous studies (Marques and Olsson 2007;
Sherwin and Glen 2003; Van Loo et al. 2005) it is likely that mice
respond to various colours. In rats, which have approximately the same
photoreceptors as mice (Lei and Yao 2006), a physiological response to
altered characteristics of the cage (perceived by humans simply as
colour changes) has been demonstrated (Dauchy et al. 2013a, 2013b,
2015; Wren et al. 2014). Therefore, researchers should remain aware of
the possibility that mice and rats may be affected by colours in ways
different from humans and in ways we are unaware. Therefore, to
provide the optimal enrichment solution in a given study,
investigators need to test the basic preferences of the animals.
Moreover, it should be investigated whether coloured items such as the
Bio-Serv Mouse Igloo® used in research affect the normal hormonal
levels involved in metabolism and physiology.
In conclusion, more than half of the female B6 mice in this study
choose to stay inside a Bio-Serv Mouse Igloo® for the majority of the
inactive period tested. When choosing an igloo the mice preferred the
blue or amber igloo over the red igloo.
References
- Dauchy, R.T., Dauchy, E.M., Hanifin, J.P., Gauthreaux, S.L., Mao, L., Belancio, V.P., Ooms, T.G., Dupepe, L.M., Jablonski, M.R., Warfield, B., Wren, M.A., Brainard, G.C., Hill, S.M., Blask, D.E., (2013a). Effects of spectral transmittance through standard laboratory cages on circadian metabolism and physiology in nude rats. The Journal of the American Association for Laboratory Animal Science. 52, 146–156.
- Dauchy, R.T., Wren, M.A., Dauchy, E.M., Hanifin, J.P., Jablonski, M.R., Warfield, B., Brainard, G.C., Hill, S.M., Mao, L., Dupepe, L.M., Ooms, T.G., Blask, D.E., (2013b). Effect of spectral transmittance through red-tinted rodent cages on circadian metabolism and physiology in nude rats. The Journal of the American Association for Laboratory Animal Science. 52, 745–755.
-
Dauchy, R.T., Hoffman, A.E., Wren-Dail, M.A., Hanifin, J.P.,
Warfield, B., Brainard, G.C., Xiang, S., Yuan, L., Hill, S.M.,
Belancio, V.P., Dauchy, E.M., Smith, K., Blask, D.E., (2015).
Daytime Blue Light Enhances the Nighttime Circadian Melatonin
Inhibition of Human Prostate Cancer Growth.
Comparative Medicine. 65, 473–485.
- Directive 2010/63/EU, (2010). Directive 2010/63/EU of The European Parliament and of the Council of 22 September 2010 on the protection of animals used for scientific purposes.
-
Howerton, C.L., Garner, J.P., Mench, J.A., (2008) Effects of a
runnings wheel-igloo enrichment on aggression, hierarchy linearity,
and stereotypy in group-housed male CD-1 (ICR) mice.
Applied Animal Behaviour Science. 115,
90–103.
-
Huberman, A.D., Niell, C.M., (2011). What can mice tell us about how
vision works? Trends in Neurosciences. 34,
464–473. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tins.2011.07.002
Jacobs, G.H., Neitz, J., Deegan, J.F., (1991). Retinal receptors in rodents maximally sensitive to ultraviolet light. Nature. 353, 655–656. https://doi.org/10.1038/353655a0
-
Key, D., (2004). Environmental enrichment options for laboratory
rats and mice. Laboratory Animals. 33,
39–44. https://doi.org/10.1038/laban0204-39
-
Kilkenny, C., Browne, W.J., Cuthill, I.C., Emerson, M., Altman,
D.G., (2010). Improving bioscience research reporting: the ARRIVE
guidelines for reporting animal research. PLOS Biology.
8, e1000412.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.1000412
-
Latham, N., Mason, G., (2004). From house mouse to mouse house: the
behavioural biology of free-living Mus musculus and its implications
in the laboratory. Applied Animal Behaviour Science.
International Society for Applied Ethology Special Issue: A
selection of papers from the 36th ISAE International Congress.
86, 261–289.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2004.02.006
-
Lei, B., Yao, G., (2006). Spectral attenuation of the mouse, rat,
pig and human lenses from wavelengths 360 nm to 1020 nm.
Experimental Eye Research. 83, 610–614.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.exer.2006.02.013
-
Marques, J.M., Olsson, I.A., (2007). The effect of preweaning and
postweaning housing on the behaviour of the laboratory mouse (Mus
musculus). Laboratory Animals. 41, 92–102.
https://doi.org/10.1258/002367707779399482
-
NASA, 2017. What Wavelength Goes With a Color? [WWW Document]. URL
https://science-edu.larc.nasa.gov/EDDOCS/Wavelengths_for_Colors.html
(accessed 5.22.17).
-
Pedersen, C.S., Sørensen, D.B., Parachikova, A.I., Plath, N.,
(2014). PCP-induced deficits in murine nest building activity:
employment of an ethological rodent behavior to mimic negative-like
symptoms of schizophrenia. Behavioural Brain Research.
273, 63–72.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2014.07.023
-
Robertson, K.L., Rowland, N.E., (2005). Effect of two types of
environmental enrichment for singly housed mice on food intake and
weight gain. Lab Animal (NY). 34, 29–32.
https://doi.org/http://dx.doi.org.ep.fjernadgang.kb.dk/10.1038/laban1005-29
-
Sager, T.N., Kirchhoff, J., Mørk, A., Van Beek, J., Thirstrup, K.,
Didriksen, M., Lauridsen, J.B., (2010). Nest building performance
following MPTP toxicity in mice.
Behavioural Brain Research. 208, 444–449.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2009.12.014
-
Sherwin, C.M., Glen, E.F., (2003). Cage colour preferences and
effects of home cage colour on anxiety in laboratory mice.
Animal Behaviour. 66, 1085–1092.
https://doi.org/10.1006/anbe.2003.2286
-
Van Loo, P.L.P., Blom, H.J.M., Meijer, M.K., Baumans, V., (2005).
Assessment of the use of two commercially available environmental
enrichments by laboratory mice by preference testing.
Laboratory Animals. 39, 58–67.
https://doi.org/10.1258/0023677052886501
-
Wren, M.A., Dauchy, R.T., Hanifin, J.P., Jablonski, M.R., Warfield,
B., Brainard, G.C., Blask, D.E., Hill, S.M., Ooms, T.G., Bohm, R.P.,
(2014). Effect of different spectral transmittances through tinted
animal cages on circadian metabolism and physiology in
Sprague-Dawley rats.
The Journal of the American Association for Laboratory
Animal Science
53, 44–51.
-
Wren-Dail, M.A., Dauchy, R.T., Ooms, T.G., Baker, K.C., Blask, D.E.,
Hill, S.M., Dupepe, L.M., Bohm, R.P., (2016). Effects of Colored
Enrichment Devices on Circadian Metabolism and Physiology in Male
Sprague-Dawley Rats. Comparative Medicine.
66, 384–391.
- Yokoyama, S., Radlwimmer, F.B., Kawamura, S., (1998). Regeneration of ultraviolet pigments of vertebrates. FEBS Letters. 423, 155–158. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0014-5793(98)00086-6